Too Short for a Blog Post, Too Long for a Tweet 108
Here's two excerpts from a book I recently read, "The Undoing Project: A Friendship That Changed Our Minds," by Michael Lewis:
Later, when he was a university professor, Danny would
tell students, “When someone says something, don’t ask yourself if it is
true. Ask what it might be true of.” That was his intellectual
instinct, his natural first step to the mental hoop: to take whatever
someone had just said to him and try not to tear it down but to make
sense of it. The question the Israeli military had asked him—Which
personalities are best suited to which military roles?—had turned out to
make no sense. And so Danny had gone and answered a different, more
fruitful question: How do we prevent the intuition of interviewers from
screwing up their assessment of army recruits? He’d been asked to divine
the character of the nation’s youth. Instead he’d found out something
about people who try to divine other people’s character: Remove their
gut feelings, and their judgments improved. He’d been handed a narrow
problem and discovered a broad truth. “The difference between Danny and
the next nine hundred and ninety-nine thousand nine hundred and
ninety-nine psychologists is his ability to find the phenomenon and then
explain it in a way that applies to other situations,” said Dale
Griffin, a psychologist at the University of British Columbia. “It looks
like luck but he keeps doing it.”
By
changing the context in which two things are compared, you submerge
certain features and force others to the surface. “It is generally
assumed that classifications are determined by similarities among the
objects,” wrote Amos, before offering up an opposing view: that “the
similarity of objects is modified by the manner in which they are
classified. Thus, similarity has two faces: causal and derivative. It
serves as a basis for the classification of objects, but is also
influenced by the adopted classification.” A banana and an apple seem
more similar than they otherwise would because we’ve agreed to call them
both fruit. Things are grouped together for a reason, but, once they
are grouped, their grouping causes them to seem more like each other
than they otherwise would. That is, the mere act of classification
reinforces stereotypes. If you want to weaken some stereotype, eliminate
the classification.
Amos’s
theory didn’t exactly contribute to the existing conversation about how
people made judgments of similarity. It took over the entire
conversation. Everyone else at the party just circled around Amos and
listened. “Amos’s approach to doing science wasn’t incremental,” said
Rich Gonzalez. “It proceeded by leaps and bounds. You find a paradigm
that is out there. You find a general proposition of that paradigm. And
you destroy it. He saw himself doing a negative style of science. He
used the word a lot: negative. This turns out to be a very powerful way
of doing social science.” That’s how Amos would begin: by undoing the
mistakes of others. As it turned out, other people had made some other
mistakes.
Comments