Too Long for a Tweet, Too Short for a Blog Post XXXIV
Here's an excerpt from a book I'm reading, "Quiet: The Power of Introverts in a World That Can't Stop Talking," by Susan Cain:
But Berns’s study also shed light on exactly why we’re such
conformists. When the volunteers played alone, the brain scans showed
activity in a network of brain regions including the occipital cortex
and parietal cortex, which are associated with visual and spatial
perception, and in the frontal cortex, which is associated with
conscious decision-making. But when they went along with their group’s
wrong answer, their brain activity revealed something very different.
Remember, what Asch wanted to know was whether people conformed despite
knowing that the group was wrong, or whether their perceptions had been
altered by the group. If the former was true, Berns and his team
reasoned, then they should see more brain activity in the
decision-making prefrontal cortex. That is, the brain scans would pick
up the volunteers deciding consciously to abandon their own beliefs to
fit in with the group. But if the brain scans showed heightened activity
in regions associated with visual and spatial perception, this would
suggest that the group had somehow managed to change the individual’s
perceptions.
That was exactly what happened—the conformists showed less
brain activity in the frontal, decision-making regions and more in the
areas of the brain associated with perception. Peer pressure, in other
words, is not only unpleasant, but can actually change your view of a
problem.
These early findings suggest that groups are like mind-altering
substances. If the group thinks the answer is A, you’re much more
likely to believe that A is correct, too. It’s not that you’re saying
consciously, “Hmm, I’m not sure, but they all think the answer’s A, so
I’ll go with that.” Nor are you saying, “I want them to like me, so I’ll
just pretend that the answer’s A.” No, you are doing something much
more unexpected—and dangerous. Most of Berns’s volunteers reported
having gone along with the group because “they thought that they had
arrived serendipitously at the same correct answer.” They were utterly
blind, in other words, to how much their peers had influenced them.
What
does this have to do with social fear? Well, remember that the
volunteers in the Asch and Berns studies didn’t always conform.
Sometimes they picked the right answer despite their peers’ influence.
And Berns and his team found something very interesting about these
moments. They were linked to heightened activation in the amygdala, a
small organ in the brain associated with upsetting emotions such as the
fear of rejection.
Berns refers to this as “the pain of independence,”
and it has serious implications. Many of our most important civic
institutions, from elections to jury trials to the very idea of majority
rule, depend on dissenting voices. But when the group is literally
capable of changing our perceptions, and when to stand alone is to
activate primitive, powerful, and unconscious feelings of rejection,
then the health of these institutions seems far more vulnerable than we
think.
Comments